Combining Love and Limits in Authoritative Parenting: A Conditional Sequence Model of Disciplinary Responses

Robert E. Larzelere, PhD
Director, Residential Research
Father Flanagan's Boys' Home, Boys Town, NE


Abstract

In contrast to the common practice of pitting love and limits against each other, several research programs have shown that optimal parenting combines the two. This paper outlines a conditional sequence model of optimal disciplinary responses and shows its consistency with a wide range of research. The model suggests that optimal disciplinary responses begin with less severe tactics, such as reasoning, but proceed to firmer disciplinary tactics when the initial tactic achieves neither compliance nor an acceptable compromise. The firmer tactics can be nonphysical punishment initially with nonabusive physical punishment reserved as a back-up for the nonphysical punishment. This is consistent with many studies showing that a combination of reasoning and punishment is more effective than either one alone and with new evidence that this sequence enhances the effectiveness of milder disciplinary tactics with preschoolers.

Childrearing advice to American parents has always been amazingly diverse, with major changes between generations and contradictory advice at any one time. The founder of behaviorism wrote a leading childrearing book in the 1920s that advocated strictness and rigidity, even warning mothers against the dangers of expressing love toward their children (Watson, 1928). Spock's Child and Baby Care (1968) introduced a better balance between love and discipline, while affirming the common sense that most parents have.

Polarization of Love and Limits

Popular Books

Current popular childrearing books often emphasize either nurturance and communication on the one hand or firm control on the other. Take, for example, the three best-selling popular books from my bookshelf. Thomas Gordon's (1975) book on Parent Effective Training emphasizes communication. Seven of his 16 chapters deal with communication. Not only does he advocate good communication, he is against forceful disciplinary tactics. "One thing [to learn] from this book," he said, is that "each and every time they force a child to do something by using their power or authority, they deny that child a chance to learn self-discipline" (p. 158). At the other extreme is James Dobson's (1970) book, Dare to Discipline. Four of his 7 chapters are on disciplinary responses to misbehavior. He is not against nurturance or communication, but the first of his five key elements is the following: "Developing respect for parents is the critical factor in child management." Elsewhere he said, "When a youngster tries . . . stiff-necked rebellion, you had better take it out of him, and pain is a marvelous purifier" (p. 16).

A third best-seller presents more of a balance between the two extremes represented by Gordon and Dobson. Fitzhugh Dodson emphasizes both nurturance and control in his title, How to Discipline with Love. Although 6 of his 9 chapters are on discipline responses, his balance is represented by the following quote: "I believe it is far better to solve a conflict by negotiation and agreement rather than through power. However, in extreme cases . . . I believe we have to fall back on sheer power" (p. 92).

Social Science

The same polarization exists in empirical, social scientific literature. Cognitive developmental psychology and behavioral parent training have had the most sustained series of empirical studies on parental discipline. They complement each other in many important ways, as shown in Table 1. However, they often hold contradictory views about optimal disciplinary responses. Cognitive developmentalists recommend reasoning as a disciplinary response (Grusec & Kuczysnki, 1997; Grusec & Goodnow, 1994; Hoffman, 1977). Their measures of parental discipline often reflect this by including reasoning and gentle discipline tactics at one end of a continuum and harsh punishment at the other end (e.g., Kochanska, 1991; Weiss, Dodge & Bates, 1992). In contrast, behavioral parent trainers feature consistent use of punishment as a response to misbehavior in training parents to manage their disruptive children more effectively (Barkley, 1987; Eyberg & Boggs, 1989; Forehand & McMahon, 1981; Patterson, 1982). They generally regard reasoning as an ineffective discipline tactic (e.g., Blum, Williams, Friman & Christopherson, 1995), except for descriptions of the contingencies of punishment and reinforcement.

Table 1. Emphases of Behavioral and Cognitive Developmental Views of Parental Socialization of Children

Issue Cognitive Behavioral

Research Methods:

Families Studied

Typical, often Middle-Class

Clinic-Referred

Design

Correlational, Longitudinal

Clinical Trials, Single-N Designs

Validity Strength

Ecological

Internal

Theoretical:

Emphases

Moral Internalization, Dispositional

Contingencies, Environmental

Issues Incorporated

Developmental Changes, Attributions

Learning Principles

Recommendations:

Foundations for Socialization

Quality of Parent-Child Relationship

Positive Reinforcement

Primary Socialization Goals

Moral Internalization, Prosocial Behavior

Reduce Anti-Social Behavior

Proactive Discipline

Explanations, Modeling

Clear Commands, Select Target Misbehaviors

Primary Discipline Response

Reasoning

Response Cost, Time-out

Other Issues

Age-Appropriate Autonomy

Functional Analysis


Adapted from Larzelere et al. (1996). Copyright 1996, Haworth Press, Used by permission.

A few experts have bridged this conceptual gap, most notably Diana Baumrind in her work on parenting styles. She contrasted three major parenting styles, authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive. Authoritarian parents emphasized firm control, permissive parents emphasized nurturance, whereas authoritative parents emphasized both. In addition, authoritative parents emphasized communication with their children and encouraged age-appropriate skills and autonomy. The children of authoritative parents generally showed more individual initiative and social responsibility in Baumrind's studies, compared to children of the other two types of parents.

The contradictory recommendations of cognitive developmental vs. clinical behavioral research concerning reasoning vs. punishment is an important puzzle that needs solved to have a good scientific foundation for advising parents. Solving that puzzle has been a major goal of my own research program. Something is wrong when the most recommended disciplinary tactic in developmental psychology is totally ignored in clinical behavioral work with parents and when the centrality of effective punishment in the clinical work is directly contradicted in the developmental literature. The two literatures generally ignore each other on this matter, with a few exceptions.

This strange situation is reflected in the kinds of research questions asked and the measures used, which often assume the correctness of the author's implicit recommendations. For example, few studies investigate differences between effective and counterproductive use of a particular disciplinary tactic, whether reasoning or punishment. Instead, the preferred disciplinary tactic is assumed to be invariably effective and the other one invariably ineffective, thus thwarting finer discriminations about their respective effectiveness. In contrast, my research program attempted to compare the effectiveness of reasoning and punishment with each other and with other disciplinary responses.

Combining Reasoning With Punishment

My most important studies focused on maternal disciplinary responses to misbehavior of 2- and 3-year-olds. Mothers were asked to record all occurrences of disobedience or fighting over a 4-week period. Using the Discipline Record (Larzelere & Merenda, 1994), they recorded the time of each incident and the discipline tactics they used to respond to it. The most common outcome variable was the delay until the next recurrence of the same kind of misbehavior. Presumably, the more effective the disciplinary response, the longer the delay until the next recurrence.

One major finding was that the combined use of reasoning and punishment was more effective in delaying misbehavior recurrences than was either one alone (see Figures 1 and 2). This was replicated for two kinds of misbehavior: fighting and disobedience. However, the evidence that this association represented a causal influence of the maternal disciplinary response on subsequent child misbehavior was stronger for fighting incidents than for disobedience incidents (Larzelere, Schneider, Larson & Pike, 1996).

bar graph
[full image: 11K]

Figure 1. Mean delay until next fighting recurrence (Larzelere et al., 1996).

bar graph
[full image: 11K]

Figure 2. Mean delay until next disobedience recurrence (Larzelere et al., 1996).

Most previous research on parental discipline has been done in such a way that it was impossible to detect the effectiveness of a reasoning-punishment combination. It has almost always been an effective disciplinary response when a research investigator looked for it. In some studies the superior effectiveness of a reasoning-punishment combination was shown in terms of immediate compliance (Chapman & Zahn-Waxler, 1982; Crockenberg & Litman, 1990; Davies, McMahon, Flessati & Tiedemann, 1984; Goodenough, 1931; Lytton & Zwirner, 1975), whereas other studies showed it in terms of moral internalization (Cheyne & Walters, 1969; Dix & Grusec, 1983; Hoffman, 1977; Israel & Brown, 1979; LaVoie, 1974; Parke, 1969). Our own study demonstrated the effectiveness in terms of subsequent misbehavior inhibition, a potential precursor of moral internalization.

Punishment Enhances Reasoning

Another advantage of a reasoning-punishment combination is that it enhances the subsequent effectiveness of reasoning when used by itself. Three different analyses of my data showed that disciplinary reasoning with 2- and 3-year-olds was ineffective unless it was backed up with punishment periodically. The children whose behavior improved the most over the next 20 months were those whose mothers frequently used reasoning alone (i.e., without punishment), but also backed up the reasoning with punishment when necessary. In contrast, the children whose behavior deteriorated the most had mothers who frequently used reasoning alone, but rarely backed it up with punishment.

The same kind of conditional sequencing of disciplinary tactics has been studied with a stronger research design by Mark Roberts and his colleagues. Behavioral parent training has been shown to be one of the most effective treatments for disruptive children (Kazdin, 1987). Roberts did a series of studies to identify the components of that treatment that accounted for its effectiveness. The treatment includes five major components: positive reinforcement of appropriate behavior, clear instructions, a single warning of impending time out, time out, and a back-up for time-out noncompliance, traditionally a 2-swat spank for 2- to 6-year-olds. With particularly noncompliant children, time out was often ineffective without a back-up procedure to enforce compliance with the time out. The spank back-up was effective in enforcing compliance with the time out, thus making the time out effective and minimizing the subsequent need for the spank back-up. Roberts and his colleagues investigated 3 alternative back-up procedures, but a brief room isolation was the only back-up that proved to be as effective as the original spank back-up. Similar to my research, contingent use of a more severe back-up procedure enhanced the subsequent effectiveness of a less severe disciplinary tactic, in this case time out.

A Conditional Sequence Model

Putting these two research programs together suggests the kind of conditional sequencing of discipline tactics as shown in Figure 3. This method of combining love and limits begins with reasoning first. If that did not achieve the disciplinary goal, then it would be backed up with time out. If the child did not comply with time out, it would be backed up with an enforcer such as a 2-swat spank or a brief room isolation.

Reasoning -> Noncorporal Punishment - > Non-Abusive Corporal Punishment

Figure 3. Simple conditional sequence model of parental disciplinary responses.

As an idealized disciplinary sequence, this conditional sequencing model has several important features. First, it is consistent with authoritative parenting, as opposed to either authoritarian or permissive parenting. Authoritative parenting uses both reasoning and firm control. Authoritarian parenting tends to skip the reasoning and go immediately to punishment. Perhaps they would also be more likely to go directly to harsher punishment without first trying a milder form of punishment. Permissive parenting is less likely to try to modify children's behavior with either reasoning or punishment.

Second, this conditional sequence model tries gentler love-motivated disciplinary responses first, followed by firm punishment only when necessary. Third, the conditional sequence model is consistent with empirical research that shows that the effectiveness of milder disciplinary tactics depends upon their being backed up by more severe disciplinary tactics when necessary (e.g., Larzelere, Sather, Schneider, Larson & Pike, 1998; Roberts & Powers, 1990).

Fourth, the conditional sequence model has the potential of reconciling the contradictory recommendations of cognitive developmentalists and behavioral clinicians. It implies that particularly disruptive children are going to need contingent punishment more often. These are the kinds of children who get referred to behavioral parent training programs. Successful use of the conditional sequence model will result in well-behaved children whose parents use reasoning effectively and rarely resort to punishment. This is consistent with the correlations that have provided the empirical support for reasoning's presumed superiority over punishment in the developmental psychology literature. Relatedly, Straus (personal communication) has reported that, in three of the five cohorts in Straus et al. (1997), the outcomes of spanking frequency depended upon the initial level of the child's antisocial behavior. Spanking frequency reduced antisocial behavior in the most antisocial children, but increased it in the least antisocial children. This is consistent with the idea that contingent punishment is particularly important for turning around the misbehavior of disruptive children, but that parents should be resorting more often to gentler tactics such as reasoning with better behaved children.

Predictions of the Model

The conditional sequence model makes a variety of predictions that are consistent with the evidence to date, some of it counter-intuitive. First, it predicts that the correlational superiority of reasoning over punishment will be more evident among easily managed children than among difficult children. This is consistent with Grusec and Goodnow's (1994) assessment of the cognitive developmental literature. Disciplinary reasoning was associated with better child outcomes than was punishment, but mostly in samples of middle-class mothers. Samples of fathers, working-class families, preschoolers, boys, and temperamentally difficult children have generally failed to find such associations. Kochanska (1991) used a measure that contrasted power assertion at one extreme with rational growth encouragement at the other extreme. She found that power assertion predicted less conscience development in high-anxiety children, but not in low-anxiety children. She concluded that there were different paths to conscience development in the two types of children. An alternative conclusion is that parents could phase out the punishment aspects of the conditional sequence model faster for high-anxiety children than for low-anxiety children. A more equal balance between reasoning and punishment would then have been optimal for the low-anxiety children, which her measure would have missed (because an equal balance was represented in the muddled middle of her measure).

A second prediction is that the superiority of punishment over reasoning will be most evident among particularly difficult children. Patterson (1982) initially tried to help parents manage their disruptive children better by emphasizing reinforcement of appropriate behaviors. By 1982, he concluded, "If I were allowed to select only one concept to use in training parents of antisocial children, I would teach them how to punish more effectively" (p. 111), referring to time out as the punishment of choice.

Thirdly, the conditional sequence model predicts that children's aggression may be decreased more by conditional recommendations against spanking than by universal recommendations against spanking. Universal anti-spanking advice may make gentler disciplinary tactics less effective unless an equally effective back-up replaces spanking. Some evidence from Sweden is consistent with that prediction. Figure 4 shows the frequency of assaults by minors against minors since Sweden banned spanking in 1979. Figure 5 shows a possible reason for that: Swedish parents are less likely to use recommended alternative disciplinary tactics such as reasoning and time out, but they are more likely to use yelling and restraining (Palmerus & Scarr, 1995). Instead, the conditional sequence model implies that spanking should be discouraged as a first response to misbehavior and as a second response to continued misbehavior, but it should not be prohibited as a back-up enforcer for nonphysical punishment, unless a replacement enforcer is used.

line graph
[full image: 15K]

Figure 4. Frequency of child abuse and assaults by minors against minors, 1981 to 1994 (Statistics Sweden, 1995).

bar graph
[full image: 20K]

Figure 5. Discipline practices reported by parents in Sweden and the United States (from Palmerus & Scarr, 1995).

This is consistent with behavioral parent training, which discourages spanking except to enforce time-out compliance. Outcome studies support the effectiveness of this treatment for reducing disruptive behavior (Kazdin, 1995). In addition, four studies found that behavioral parent training reduced parental spanking. Two of them used the spank back-up for noncompliance with time out (Eyberg, 1993; Roberts, 1984), and two of them used alternative back-ups for time out (McNeil, Clemens-Mowrer, Gurwitch & Funderburk, 1994; Webster-Stratton, 1990). How spanking is used is more critical than whether it is used.

A fourth prediction of the conditional sequence model is that early prevention programs that discourage the use of parental punishment will be more effective for general populations than for at-risk populations. Guterman (1997) reviewed the 18 most rigorous evaluations of early parenting programs to prevent child abuse. He found that 8 of the 10 programs targeted to general populations were effective in reducing subsequent child abuse, but only 1 of the 8 programs for at-risk populations showed similar effectiveness. The programs all featured support services and parent education. Presumably the support services would help any family, but the parent education component may have helped parents deal with easily managed children more than it helped with at-risk children. I am assuming that the parent education components often discouraged punishment, but I cannot document that.

Finally, the conditional sequence model predicts that features of the model will minimize the risk of escalation within discipline incidents. Some support for this is shown by the evidence summarized above that behavioral parent training decreases subsequent use of spanking. It also seems consistent with the escalation processes differentiating aggressive from non-aggressive families, as identified by Snyder and his colleagues (Snyder, Edwards, McGraw, Kilgore & Holton, 1994). They found that parents of aggressive sons tended to match their sons' level of aversiveness, whereas parents of non-aggressive sons were more likely to respond at a slightly lower level of aversiveness. Further, parents of aggressive sons were more likely to respond to any de-escalation of the conflict by escalating the incident themselves. In contrast, parents of non-aggressive sons were likely to respond to the child's de-escalation by bringing the incident to a close. Thus parents of non-aggressive sons out-persist their children, but do not out-escalate them. In one sense, this may seem to contradict the conditional sequence model, implying that parents should never escalate to spanking. As discussed above, however, the spank back-up enhances subsequent effectiveness of less severe disciplinary tactics, helping persistence to work later without resorting to spanking. Parents should typically persist until either compliance is achieved or the child de-escalates the situation in an appropriate way (e.g., specifying when he will do the requested chore). They should prefer the least aversive tactics that will accomplish that end, but be willing to resort to more aversive, but non-abusive disciplinary tactics when necessary.

Expanding the Model

To this point, this paper has focused on a simple three-step conditional sequence model. Although it suggests a way to reconcile contradictory recommendations and successfully makes several innovative predictions, actual parental practices are more varied. This section uses empirical evidence to suggest helpful ways to expand the simple model. First, there is some evidence that a single warning can dramatically reduce the need to move to the next level of severity without compromising the effectiveness of the overall disciplinary strategy. Roberts (1982) found that a single time-out warning reduced the need to use time out by 74% without diminishing the effectiveness of the overall parental disciplinary sequence. Multiple warnings reduce the effectiveness of the overall discipline somewhat (Sherrill, O'Leary & Kendziora, 1992).

The tactics used can also be expanded beyond the three featured in the simple conditional sequence model. My major research was designed to investigate a wide range of disciplinary responses with 2- and 3-year-olds. As indicated above, the combination of reasoning and punishment was associated with the longest delay until the next recurrence of misbehavior. Some other disciplinary responses, however, were nearly as effective as the reasoning-punishment combination. Four of 13 other discipline responses were never significantly worse in delaying misbehavior recurrences. Three were combinations of an aversive verbal disciplinary response with some kind of action. Aversive verbal tactics included scolding or shaming, threatening, and yelling. The actions were either punishment, forced compliance, or promoting positive behavior. Aversive models without one of these action components were significantly less effective than other discipline responses. The remaining effective disciplinary response was a combination of reasoning and forced compliance. Thus five of six possible combinations of a verbal disciplinary tactic with an action component were the five most effective disciplinary responses at delaying the next misbehavior recurrence.

The most promising of these is the combination of reasoning and forced compliance. Like the reasoning-punishment combination, it tended to delay the next misbehavior recurrence more than did either forced compliance or reasoning alone. However, the combination of forced compliance and reasoning did not enhance the subsequent effectiveness of reasoning as consistently as the reasoning-punishment combination did. One possible reason is that punishment communicates to the child that he or she should have made a better behavior choice. The reasoning component clarifies what that choice should have been and why. In contrast, forced compliance communicates that the parent will rescue the child from difficulty whenever he or she gets entangled in a problem, thus minimizing the need for the child to make a better choice in the future. Nonetheless, the combination of reasoning with forced compliance seems to be an effective disciplinary response even though it is limited in enhancing the subsequent effectiveness of reasoning.

The combinations of aversive verbal tactics with actions seem problematic despite their effectiveness. Taken to excess, aversive verbal tactics should not be recommended. Some findings that affective intensity enhances the effectiveness of reasoning might help account for this somewhat (Chamberlain, Reid, Ray, Capaldi & Fisher, 1997; Pfiffner & O'Leary, 1989). Zahn-Waxler, Radke-Yarrow, and King (1979) explored the affective intensity of maternal explanations to 15- to 29-month-old preschoolers in some detail. They found that verbal explanations were used in 40% of disciplinary incidents. In most of these cases, the mothers communicated intensely with moralizing and judgmental components. Affectively and morally charged explanations were positively associated with child reparations and altruism. Neutrally communicated explanations were the only form of explanations that failed to correlate positively with child reparations. Similarly, one of our earlier studies found that, when reasoning alone resulted in no child distress, it was a less effective disciplinary response than when it resulted in some child distress for about a minute or two (Larzelere & Merenda, 1994).

Perhaps the affective intensity is the aspect of aversive verbal tactics that make them effective when combined with an action component. Further research is needed to clarify when such verbal intensity becomes counterproductive.

Sather (1992) examined whether the effectiveness of other verbal disciplinary tactics was enhanced by being backed up with punishment. He found some evidence that each verbal tactic was subsequently more effective after being combined with punishment. The verbal tactics included scold/shame or yell, threaten, label behavior bad, and commanding. The effectiveness of a punishment back-up for enhancing subsequent use of the verbal tactic was found for both disobedience and fighting incidents for labeling the behavior bad, but only for one type of incident for the other four verbal tactics.

I have recently investigated whether other forceful backup procedures would also enhance the subsequent effectiveness of reasoning. One obvious alternative is forced compliance. As summarized above, it rarely enhanced the subsequent effectiveness of reasoning by itself, perhaps because of different motivational implications than is the case for punishment.

Another conclusion from my studies concerns the level of child distress associated with maximal effectiveness. Research on punishment has found that the stronger the punishment, the more effective it is. That suggests that effectiveness will be maximized when the child is most distressed. That is what we found when punishment was used alone (Larzelere & Merenda, 1994). However, when reasoning was used, whether by itself or in combination with punishment, an intermediate level of child distress was associated with optimal effectiveness. This is consistent with previous research that found that adding reasoning to punishment changed two important parameters of punishment's effectiveness. Once reasoning was added to punishment, its effectiveness no longer depended on its severity. Second, punishment's effectiveness no longer depended upon its precise timing once reasoning was added to it (Cheyne &Walters, 1969; Parke, 1969). As will be seen, this pattern is also consistent with Hoffman's (1977) theory of moral internalization.

Related Theories

There have been several theories proposed to explain the presumed greater effectiveness of reasoning in developmental psychology. Three of these will be briefly considered for their consistency with the conditional sequence model. Hoffman (1977) posited that all disciplinary responses have an element of power assertion and an element of reasoning, which he called induction. The reasoning component was most crucial for moral internalization. Its effectiveness was influenced by combining it with the right amount of power assertion. Parents needed to use just enough power assertion for the child to attend to the reasoning component, but not so much as to undermine the child's cognitive processing capability.

Hoffman's theory predicts that an intermediate level of power assertion would maximize the effectiveness of reasoning as a disciplinary tactic, which Larzelere and Merenda (1994) found. The finding that reasoning was associated with better child behavior than was punishment had been the primary empirical support for the theory previously. The effectiveness of reasoning is greatest at an intermediate level of child distress when reasoning is combined with punishment, but not when it is combined with forced compliance (Larzelere & Merenda, 1994;Merenda, 1992). This provides a little support for the motivational differences between punishment and forced compliance described above. Hoffman has not taken into account the effect of previous conditional sequences of disciplinary tactics on how current disciplinary tactics are associated with moral internalization. The conditional sequence model suggests that children will attend more to disciplinary reasoning if their parents have previously backed up reasoning with punishment when necessary.

Mark Lepper (1983) has used attribution theory to explain the supposed greater effectiveness of mild disciplinary responses compared to severe disciplinary responses. It seems to be a plausible theory of moral internalization even though several replicated findings provide evidence against it. The contradictory evidence includes the fact that the combination of reasoning and punishment is more effective than reasoning alone and that reasoning is more effective when parents communicate more intensely, thus raising the level of child distress a little bit.

Lepper's theory assumes that children make attributions about their own behavior in the same way that adults do. Children do not fully develop these tendencies until about age 7. In particular, they do not use the discounting principle to explain other people's behavior until then. The discounting principle implies that internal reasons for good behavior (for oneself and others) gets discounted to the extent that there are external reasons for that behavior (e.g., an overbearing, punitive parent). Unlike adults, preschool children use the additive principle, that external reasons enhance attributed internal motivations for good behavior. Thus parental punishment will not undermine moral internalization in children until they are about 7 years of age. The conditional sequence model fits right into those developmental changes. Punishment has a role during the preschool years when it would not undermine moral internalization. But its major role should be to enforce gentler disciplinary tactics such as reasoning, preparing for the day when mild disciplinary responses will be optimal for moral internalization. Lepper's theory assumes that gentler disciplinary responses will be as effective in gaining compliance as more forceful responses. The conditional sequence model suggests how gentler disciplinary responses come to have the effectiveness necessary for them to facilitate moral internalization.

Finally, Bell's (1986) control system model overlaps with the conditional sequence model in important ways. A classic article by Bell (1968) had argued that most associations between parental disciplinary tactics and child behavior could reflect the child's influence on the parent rather than the parent's influence on the child. The control system model goes beyond that by focusing on how parents and children regulate each other in ways similar to a thermostat. When children's behavior gets too inappropriate, then parents respond with upper limit controls to reduce or redirect such excess behavior. When children are too shy and withdrawn, then parents respond with lower limit controls to prime or stimulate appropriate behavior. To extend this model further, parents may have yet another set of responses to maintain or direct appropriate child behavior in between upper and lower limit controls.

The relevance of Bell's theory for the conditional sequence model is that the punishment tactics may need to be used more often as upper limit controls than as maintenance responses or lower limit controls. Punishment will be elicited more often by children who act out, a conclusion consistent with a wide range of research. Whether punishment is more effective with more aggressive children is debatable, but there are some supporting lines of evidence. First is the finding that children referred for disruptive disorders are helped by a behavioral parent training that emphasizes contingent punishment. A second piece of evidence is from the Straus et al. (1997) data set. In three of the five cohorts, the apparent effect of spanking frequency depended upon the initial level of antisocial behavior (Straus, 1997, personal communication). For children initially high on antisocial behavior, spanking frequency reduced their subsequent level of antisocial behavior. For children who were lowest on antisocial behavior, spanking frequency increased their subsequent level of antisocial behavior. In terms of Bell's control system model, this suggests that punishment is more effective as an upper limit control than as a maintenance or lower limit control. Consistent with the conditional sequence model, parents should be looking for opportunities to use gentler disciplinary responses when they can. The Straus et al. evidence suggests that frequent spanking becomes counterproductive with relatively well behaved children.

In conclusion, this paper has outlined a conditional sequence model of optimal disciplinary responses, along with supporting evidence. In focusing so much on disciplinary responses, this paper has ignored many important aspects of positive parenting. Table 1 suggests some important aspects of the larger parenting context.

The major focus of this paper, however, has been on optimal ways to combine love and limits in disciplinary responses. This fits Baumrind's classic work on authoritative parenting. Table 2 summarizes a range of specific parenting practices that were significantly more characteristic of authoritative parents than of either authoritarian or permissive parents (Baumrind, 1967). Many of these characteristics are consistent with the conditional sequence model. Other characteristics go beyond it in important ways, particularly in the specifics of nurturance and maturity demands. This paper must be considered within the larger parenting context. Nurturance, age-appropriate autonomy, skill development, family routines, and family rituals are some of the characteristics of optimal parent-child relationships. These characteristics are important in their own right and also important for setting the stage for optimal disciplinary responses. Those responses should reflect the best balance of love and firmness. Hopefully, this paper will help parents and parent educators clarify that appropriate balance.

Table 2. Significant Differences Between Authoritative Parents vs. Authoritarian and Permissive Parents

Parenting Measure vs.
Authoritarian
vs.
Permissive

Parental Control
Positive outcome by persistence *
Doesn't give in to whining * *
Avoids evasion as response to child *
Less disciplinary friction *
Coordination & clarity of household rules *
Effectiveness of rules *
Source of power not disguised *
Strictness
Care of family property (F) *
Neatness (F) *
Orderliness responsibilities (M/F) * *
Negative sanctions
Corporal punishment
Less love withdrawal (M) *
Less ridicule (M) *
Less frightening the child (M) *
Maturity Demands
In teaching * *
In play * *
Independence training, control *
Independence training, non-control *
Less babying *
Reward self-sufficiency (M) *
Introduces new experiences (F) *
Perceives child's individuality (M) *
Communication
Respects child's decision * *
Uses reason to get compliance * *
Encourages verbal give and take * *
Nurturance
Satisfies child * *
Supports child *
Uses positive reinforcement *
Acceptance of child *
Expression of affection *
Warmth
Loving relationship (M) *
Approval (M) *
Empathy (M) *
Sympathy (M) *

(M) = mothers only (F) = fathers only (M/F) = first column for mothers, second for fathers.

* [underlined p] < .05 in Baumrind (1967)


References

Barkley, R. A. (1987). Defiant children: A clinician's manual for parent training. New York: Guilford.

Baumrind, D. (1967). Child care practices anteceding three patterns of preschool behavior. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 75, 43-88.

Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization. Psychological Review, 75, 81-95.

Bell, R. Q., & Chapman, M. (1986). Child effects in studies using experimental or brief longitudinal approaches to socialization. Developmental Psychology, 22, 595-603.

Blum, N. J., Williams, G. E., Friman, P. C., & Christopherson, E. R. (1995). Disciplining young children: The role of verbal instructions and reasoning. Pediatrics, 96, 336-341.

Chamberlain, P., Reid, J. B., Ray, J., Capaldi, D., & Fisher, P. (1997). DSM review for parent inadequate discipline (PID). In T. Widiger (Ed.), DSM-IV sourcebook . Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Chapman, M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1982). Young children's compliance and noncompliance to parental discipline in a natural setting. International Journal of Behavioural Development, 5, 81-94.

Cheyne, J. A., & Walters, R. H. (1969). Intensity of punishment, timing of punishment, and cognitive structure as determinants of response inhibition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 10, 352-364.

Crockenberg, S., & Litman, C. (1990). Autonomy as competence in two-year-olds: Maternal correlates of child defiance, compliance, and self-assertion. Developmental Psychology,, 26, 961-971.

Davies, G. R., McMahon, R. J., Flessati, E. W., & Tiedemann, G. L. (1984). Verbal rationales and modeling as adjuncts to a parenting technique for child compliance. Child Development, 55, 1290-1298.

Dix, T., & Grusec, J. E. (1983). Parental influence techniques: An attributional analysis. Child Development, 54, 645-652.

Dobson, J. (1970). Dare to discipline. Wheaton, IL: Tyndale.

Eyberg, S. (1993). The spank back-up in time-out with preschool children. Unpublished paper, University of Florida.

Eyberg, S. M., & Boggs, S. R. (1989). Parent training for oppositional-defiant preschoolers. In C. E. Schaefer & J. M. Briesmeister (Eds.), Handbook of parent training: Parents as co-therapists for children's behavior problems (pp. 105-132). New York: Wiley.

Forehand, R. L., & McMahon, R. J. (1981). Helping the noncompliant child. New York: Guilford.

Goodenough, F. L. (1931). Anger in young children. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

Gordon, T. (1975). Parent effectiveness training: The tested new way to raise responsible children. New York: Guilford Press.

Grusec, J., & Kuczysnki, L. (Eds.). (1997). Parenting and children's internalization of values. New York: Wiley.

Grusec, J. E., & Goodnow, J. J. (1994). Impact of parental discipline methods on the child's internalization of values: A reconceptualization of current points of view. Developmental Psychology, 30, 4-19.

Guterman, N. B. (1997). Early prevention of physical child abuse and neglect: Existing evidence and future directions. Child Maltreatment, 2, 12-34.

Hoffman, M. L. (1977). Moral internalization: Current theory and research. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 85-133). New York: Academic Press.

Israel, A. C., & Brown, M. S. (1979). Effects of directiveness of instructions and surveillance on the production and persistence of children's donations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 27, 250-261.

Kazdin, A. E. (1987). Treatment of antisocial behavior in children: Current status and future directions. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 187-203.

Kazdin, A. E. (1995). Conduct disorders in childhood and adolescence. (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Kochanska, G. (1991). Socialization and temperament in the development of guilt and conscience. Child Development, 62, 1379-1392.

Larzelere, R. E., & Merenda, J. A. (1994). The effectiveness of parental discipline for toddler misbehavior at different levels of child distress. Family Relations, 43, 480-488.

Larzelere, R. E., Sather, P. R., Schneider, W. N., Larson, D. B., & Pike, P. L. (1998). Punishment enhances reasoning's effectiveness as a disciplinary response to toddlers. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 388-403.

Larzelere, R. E., Schneider, W. N., Larson, D. B., & Pike, P. L. (1996). The effects of discipline responses in delaying toddler misbehavior recurrences. Child & Family Behavior Therapy, 18, 35-57.

LaVoie, J. C. (1974). Aversive, cognitive, and parental determinants of punishment generalization in adolescent males. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 124, 29-39.

Lepper, M. R. (1983). Social control processes and the internalization of social values: An attributional perspective. In E. T. Higgins, D. N. Ruble, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), Social cognition and social development: A sociocultural perspective (pp. 294-330). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Lytton, H., & Zwirner, W. (1975). Compliance and its controlling stimuli observed in a natural setting. Developmental Psychology, 11, 769-779.

McNeil, C. B., Clemens-Mowrer, L., Gurwitch, R. H., & Funderburk, B. W. (1994). Assessment of a new procedure to prevent timeout escape in preschoolers. Child & Family Behavior Therapy, 16(3), 27-35.

Merenda, J. A. (1992). The impact of child distress on the effectiveness of reasoning as a discipline response with toddlers: Testing Hoffman's socialization theory. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Biola University, La Mirada, CA.

Palmerus, K., & Scarr, S. (1995, April). How parents discipline young children: Cultural comparisons and individual differences. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, IN.

Parke, R. D. (1969). Effectiveness of punishment as an interaction of intensity, timing, agent nurturance, and cognitive structuring. Child Development, 40, 213-235.

Patterson, G. R. (1982). Coercive family process. Eugene, OR: Castalia Press.

Pfiffner, L. J., & O'Leary, S. G. (1989). Effects of maternal discipline and nurturance on toddlers' behavior and affect. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17, 527-540.

Roberts, M. W. (1982). The effects of warned versus unwarned time-out procedures on child noncompliance. Child & Family Behavior Therapy, 4, 37-53.

Roberts, M. W. (1984). An attempt to reduce timeout resistance in young children. Behavior Therapy, 15, 210-216.

Roberts, M. W., & Powers, S. W. (1990). Adjusting chair timeout enforcement procedures for oppositional children. Behavior Therapy, 21, 257-271.

Sather, P. R. (1992). Negative and positive side effects of alternative parental discipline responses to toddler misbehavior. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Biola University, La Mirada, CA.

Sherrill, J. T., O'Leary, S. G., & Kendziora, K. T. (1992, November). The effects of warnings and response cost on classroom behavior. Paper presented at the Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Boston.

Snyder, J., Edwards, P., McGraw, K., Kilgore, K., & Holton, A. (1994). Escalation and reinforcement in mother-child conflict: Social processes associated with the development of physical aggression. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 305-321.

Spock, B. (1968). Baby and Child Care. (Rev. ed.). New York: Pocket Books.

Straus, M. A., Sugarman, D. B., & Giles-Sims, J. (1997). Spanking by parents and subsequent antisocial behavior of children. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 151, 761-767.

Watson, J. B. (1928). Psychological care of infant and child. New York: W. W. Norton & Co.

Webster-Stratton, C. (1990). Enhancing the effectiveness of self-administered videotape parent training for families with conduct-problem children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 479-492.

Weiss, B., Dodge, K. A., & Bates, J. E. (1992). Some consequences of early harsh discipline: Child aggression and a maladaptive social information processing style. Child Development, 63, 1321-1335.

Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., & King, R. (1979). Child rearing and children's prosocial initiations toward victims of distress. Child Development, 50, 319-330.

Copyright © 1998 Robert Larzelere.

stars icon For technical assistance:
Library Technology Group
General Library System
University of Wisconsin-Madison